Sexual Orientation and Race Intersectionally Reduce the Perceived Gendered Nature of Normative Stereotypes in the United States

  • Hudson, Sa-kiera T. J.
  • Ghani, Asma
Psychology of Women Quarterly 48(1):p 56-79, March 2024. | DOI: 10.1177/03616843231187851

There is substantial research on the nature of gender prescriptive and proscriptive stereotypes. However, there has been relatively little work on whether these normative stereotypes are equally attributed to men and women of different identities. Across two studies (total N = 928), we assessed the extent to which stereotypes are prescribed and proscribed for men and women of different sexual orientations (Study 1) and races (Study 2) in the United States. We asked participants to rate the desirability of possessing 70 traits based on an “average American.” Although results showed the persistence of gender normative stereotypes in society, the normative nature of these stereotypes was influenced by sexual orientation and race. There was strong evidence of a heterocentric bias, as normative stereotypes of generic men and women most closely aligned with those of straight men and women. There was weaker evidence of a Eurocentric bias. Furthermore, observed gender differences in normative stereotypes were significantly smaller for sexually- and racially-minoritized targets compared to straight and White targets. These findings combined suggest that the practices and policies that attempt to address gender inequality might not be as effective for people with multiply-marginalized identities that face distinctly different patterns of normative pressures. Online slides for instructors who want to use this article for teaching are available on PWQ's website athttps://journals.sagepub.com/doi/suppl/10.1177/03616843231187851.

For decades, social scientists have documented the persistent associations between women and traits like “warm,” “kind,” and “emotional,” as well as between men and traits such as “dominant,” “independent,” and “competitive” (; ; ; ; ). These gendered associations, or stereotypes, simultaneously describe the prevailing perceptions of what men and women actually do (i.e., descriptive) as well as the norms (i.e., normative) that govern what men and women should (i.e., prescriptive) and should not (i.e., proscriptive) do (). Both descriptive and normative stereotypes contribute to a lack of women's representation in men-dominated fields (; ); however, normative stereotypes are also a major source of backlash discrimination. As an example, not only are women perceived to be warm and not agentic, they should be warm and should not be agentic—making an agentic woman undesirable and subjected to punitive measures (; ).

Despite the abundant scholarship on the nature and consequences of gendered stereotypes, it is still unclear whether findings around gender normative stereotypes are equally generalizable to men and women from different social identities. Existing literature primarily investigates stereotypes of “men” or “women” without specifying social identities such as age, sexual orientation, race/ethnicity, or religion. This approach tacitly assumes generalizability to all majoritized and minoritized subgroups within the broader gender category. We say tacit because researchers do not often state that the (lack of) diversity and representation within study materials itself might be a potentially large limitation on the generalizability of their findings.

In the present research, we focus on two relevant social identities—sexual orientation and race—that have been shown to interact with gender to influence important interpersonal and intergroup outcomes such as identity development and socialization, type of discriminatory experiences, and the content of categorization (; ; ; ). Here we argue that in the case of normative stereotypes, explicitly specifying the race and sexual orientation of the investigated men and women targets will change the prescriptive and proscriptive nature of the associated stereotypes. In situations where multiple identities are relevant and salient, one cannot simply extrapolate from the normative pressures of the superordinate categories to the subordinate ones. We are using the term “race” exclusively in this manuscript although the categories often used in cited research and in our Study 2 encompass both racial (e.g., Black), and ethnic (e.g., Latino) designations. We believe this maps onto the everyday understandings of race and ethnicity (), as these distinctions might largely be academic in nature.

By interrogating the underlying assumptions around sexual orientation and race in gender research, we ask a question grounded in intersectionality, or the acknowledgment and incorporation of the interconnected nature of social identities into theories, hypotheses, methods, and interpretations (; ; ). Present in early sociological and critical feminist writings, it gained voice when coined by Kimberlé Crenshaw as a theory to understand how systems of oppression (e.g., sexism/patriarchy, racism/White supremacy) do not work independently but overlap to influence social inequality. In other words, one should not, and cannot, understand gender inequality without explicitly considering other systems of inequality.

As intersectionality work has become more common in psychology, there have been an increasing number of calls to action by scholars to address the dearth of research that examine more than one social group at a time (; ; ; ). We answered this challenge with two critiques—one methodological and one theoretical—to normative stereotype research in the realm of gender. Methodologically, we argue that ostensibly neutral labels such as “men” and “women” are not, in fact, neutral and are influenced by the cognitive biases of perceivers. Thus, the work on gender normative stereotypes likely best describes perceptions of majoritized targets—for example, White, straight individuals—and is less likely to describe perceptions of those with other identity combinations.

The methodological critique impacts the subsequent generalizability of research on gender normative stereotypes, such as when and how violations of normative expectations occur, forming our theoretical critique. If, as we hypothesize, normative expectations do differ for men and women of different sexual orientations and races, this suggests that these groups have fluctuating prescriptions and proscriptions across contexts that can cause additional burdens when navigating expectations. For these groups, the normative pressures they are under when just their race, their gender, or their sexual orientation is highlighted does not necessarily aggregate to the pressures that form when multiple identities are salient and relevant. These groups face multiple normative pressures that could even conflict in certain situations (). In contrast, the situations that can activate backlash for majoritized groups are likely relatively stable across contexts, as for a White straight individual, for example, the normative representations of their race (i.e., a White man and woman) and sexual orientation (i.e., a straight man and woman) are in line with overarching representations of their gender categories (i.e., man and woman). Thus, whether their race, sexual orientation, or combinations of the two are salient, the normative pressures they face are in sync with their gender.

In short, theories — such as social role theory and role congruity theory (; ; ; ) — that aim to understand and mitigate the impact of gendered normative stereotypes might be limited in their scope to groups that are perceived to be socially dominant, which would be heterosexual White men and women within the context of the current research. These theories also might be limited to contexts in which singular identities are the lens through which intersectional targets are perceived and evaluated (); for example, contexts where Asian women are primarily viewed as “women” but not “Asian” or “Asian women.”

Cognitive Biases in Gender Normative Stereotypes

Below we review research on the prevalence of gender-, sexuality-, and race-based cognitive biases. More specifically, cognitive biases such as androcentrism (i.e., the assumption that an unspecified person is a man), heterocentrism (i.e., the assumption that an unspecified person is straight), and Eurocentrism (i.e., the assumption that an unspecified person in the [U.S.] is White) often influence the representations generated when calling to mind a “person,” “woman,” or “man” (; , ; ; ; ; ). As an example, words that purportedly activate representations that are inclusive of both men and women (e.g., chairmen) implicitly activate only masculine concepts (). Relatedly, people show a heterocentric bias in perception, assuming heterosexuality even when the base rates suggest otherwise (). Finally, perceptions of people as White permeate many domains of cognitive representations, including leadership (; ) and the color of God's skin ().

These biases also influence stereotypes of groups. For example, descriptive stereotypes of racial and sexual minorities do not always conform to the descriptive stereotypes of the relevant majority group, leading to unique descriptive stereotypes for minorities (). As an example, using a free-response paradigm, showed that descriptive stereotypes of racial groups exhibited androcentric biases, such that the descriptive stereotypes spontaneously generated for broad racial categories (e.g., Black people or White people) most closely matched the descriptive stereotypes generated for men in that racial category (e.g., Black and White men) than the women (e.g., Black and White women). This pattern was mirrored for Eurocentric biases, as the descriptive stereotypes generated for the category of “men” and “women” most closely matched those generated for White men and women.

Descriptive stereotypes of lesbian women and gay men also do not conform to the descriptive stereotypes of their straight counterparts. The literature suggests that most people hold assumptions of gender inversion or the assumption that gay men and straight women are similar in traits, while lesbian women and straight men are similar because the target of their sexual attraction is the same (, , ; ; ). Thus, gay men are seen as possessing feminine and lesbian women are seen as possessing masculine characteristics and preferences, suggesting that traditional gender descriptive stereotypes might be limited to heterosexual men and women.

Finally, biases influence stereotypes and perceptions in an intersectional way, as certain sub-groups are foregrounded to a greater extent in the social milieu (of the U.S.) than others along multiple dimensions (). For example, Black women do not activate the category “women” as strongly as White women nor the category “Black” as strongly as Black men (; ; ) in various categorization tasks. Follow-up research has corroborated the basic finding that “Black” is seen as masculine in the U.S. society (; ) and extended it to include U.S. Asian-Americans (, ) who are presumed feminine, suggesting that gender can influence perceptions of race beyond androcentrism.

Normative Stereotypes at the Intersections

In this paper, we extend past work by asking whether normative stereotypes are also influenced by androcentric, heterocentric, and Eurocentric biases. As stated earlier, prescriptive and proscriptive stereotypes are uniquely associated with backlash discrimination when people violate the normative expectations placed on them. Although there is often an equivalence between descriptive and prescriptive expectations—how we anticipate people to be is also how we believe they should be ()—almost all of the work done to date on intersectional stereotypes has been on descriptive stereotypes.

There is reason to believe that in contrast to majoritized groups, normative stereotypes for men and women at the intersection of minoritized identities will not reflect their descriptive counterparts. For example, gay men and lesbian women can face discrimination precisely because they violate expectations placed upon their gender (). If true, that suggests that a gay man who acts femininely will be in line with descriptive expectations based on his sexual orientation but violate prescriptive expectations of masculinity based on his gender. Thus, femininity for gay men would be proscribed. However, a feminine gay man might be acting exactly as expected because he is gay, keeping descriptive expectations in line with normative ones. From this perspective, femininity for gay men would be prescribed and not punished. It is currently unclear whether femininity is prescribed or proscribed in gay men, underlying the main point that it is necessary to directly assess normative stereotypes when both gender and sexual orientation are foregrounded to address these competing deductive conclusions.

Similarly, while Black men descriptively are seen as hyper-masculine (), the backlash they face based on that masculinity () suggests that normatively in some contexts, they should not be masculine at all. The descriptive stereotype of masculinity might not be prescribed for Black men but instead proscribed, although masculinity is prescribed for the broader “man” category (). Indeed, Black men who are perceived as less masculinely, by removing their beards (), acting communally (), or even being gay () can either face less discrimination than Black men who are perceived more masculinely, or qualitatively different forms of discrimination that vary by kind rather than degree. As another example, Asian men are stereotyped as effeminate, small, and feminine () but often react in hegemonically masculine ways to counteract that stereotype (). Such behaviors are in line with a normative expectation that while their group is stereotyped as feminine, they are not supposed to act that way.

These examples highlight the importance of directly investigating normative stereotypes for intersectional targets, as descriptive and normative stereotypes might not be congruent for minoritized groups. Further, as minoritized identities are not always foregrounded during perception, assessing how normative pressures change when various aspects of intersectional identity are made salient is critical. Finally, backlash can occur not only when one violates normative expectations that get activated in a given situation (i.e., a woman running for political office versus applying to be an elementary school teacher) but also when one violates expectations in comparison to other groups. Thus, knowing the relative-normative stereotypes for intersectional groups is necessary to predict when, for example, a Black man might be punished for being masculine. A Black man might be punished for being masculine in general or perhaps only when directly compared to a less masculine White man. Indeed, White men's understanding of their masculinity can be directly tied to Black men's masculinity, as when a White man's masculinity is threatened, shooting and killing a Black man can increase it ().

From a practical application standpoint, understanding the unique normative pressures of individuals at the intersection of identity might help elucidate the boundaries of recommendations to mitigate unequal gendered treatment. For example, recommendations for women to “lean in” and assert themselves () have been criticized for being tone-deaf to women of color () and a potentially hazardous “one size fits all” strategy. Work on descriptive stereotypes suggests that the assumption that women are not agentic is nuanced by race, as only White women are seen as non-agentic from both a competence and a dominance standpoint (). In contrast, Black women are seen to possess traits in line with agentic dominance but not agentic competence, implying that “leaning in” might lead to different repercussions for Black women compared to White women. However, to fully understand those repercussions, normative expectations are also needed. Extending this example, if Black women are normatively perceived to be assertive and dominant when both race and gender are salient, that would suggest that in these circumstances, Black women could be rewarded rather than punished for dominant behavior (). Considering both descriptive and normative expectations in tandem can give a starting place to help contextualize when “leaning in” would be beneficial or detrimental to women of color.

The Current Investigation

In light of the aforementioned cognitive biases, we should not assume that gendered normative stereotypes are equally normative for men and women of other sexual orientations and races beyond straight and White. Given normative stereotypes’ role in engendering backlash, it is important to understand how people, and society at large, desire men and women of different social identities to behave (). This paper adds to the discussion of gender dynamics by providing an unfiltered view of the landscape of gender normative stereotypes for intersectional targets by sexual orientation (Study 1) and race (Study 2). We do so by soliciting “average American” perceptions of the normativity of 70 stereotypes for straight and sexually minoritized targets in Study 1 and for Asian, Black, Latino, Middle-Eastern, and White targets in Study 2.

This design allows us to not only directly compare specific normative stereotypes across a range of groups, but to also be in conversation with previous work that assessed stereotypes on the “average American” level (see ; ). It is important to note, however, that assessing stereotypes from the point of view of the “average American” is still limiting because of the prototypicality biases mentioned earlier. As “average” U.S. Americans are likely seen to be White, cis-gender men, we are assessing perceptions of intersectional groups that have been colored by this influence. That being said, we hope this paper will serve as a useful reference for future work on intersectionality as well as a place to generate hypotheses that extend and nuance existing research on gender.

Hypotheses

We had three major hypotheses regarding the nature of intersectional normative gender stereotypes. First, we hypothesized that despite increases in women's representation in men-dominated spaces, gender normative stereotypes still exist (), an expectation that has been corroborated by recent longitudinal and big-data work on gender descriptive stereotypes (; ). Thus, we expected people to believe a “woman” (without any identity qualifiers) should display traditionally feminine and not masculine traits while a “man” (without any identity qualifiers) should have traditionally masculine and not feminine traits. However, these gendered normative stereotypes are rooted in biased beliefs about women and men. Thus, our second hypothesis was that cognitive biases such as androcentrism, heterocentrism, and Eurocentrism, would influence normative stereotypes. We expected stereotypes of targets labeled as a “person” to be more similar to stereotypes of targets labeled as a “man” compared to a “woman” (i.e., androcentrism; Studies 1 and 2). We also expected stereotypes of unlabeled targets to be more similar to stereotypes of targets labeled as “straight” than “gay” (i.e., heterocentrism; Study 1), as well as “White” than any other race (i.e., Eurocentrism; Study 2).

Our third hypothesis was that sexual orientation and race would substantively alter the normative nature of stereotypes for men and women. Since, to our knowledge, there was no existing work on intersectional normative stereotypes, this hypothesis was exploratory. However, we outline what would be expected if normative stereotypes are similar to descriptive ones () for intersectional targets. At the intersection of sexual orientation and gender (Study 1), we would expect gender inversion () to drive stereotype expectations such that gender differences between gay men and lesbian women's normative stereotypes would be the inverse of gender differences between straight men and women's. In other words, normative stereotypes of gay men would be more similar to straight women's than straight men's stereotypes, while normative stereotypes of lesbian women would be more similar to straight men's than straight women's stereotypes.

At the intersection of race and gender, we again hypothesized that normative stereotypes would be similar to descriptive ones based on the gendered race hypothesis (; ; ). Thus, Black men and women were expected to face amplified prescriptions of masculinity while Asian men and women were expected to face amplified prescriptions of femininity. To add to the literature on racial stereotypes more broadly, we investigated normative gender stereotypes of several of the largest racial groups in the U.S., including Asian, Black, Latino, Middle-Eastern, and White. However, research on stereotypes of racial groups beyond Black and Asian, in general, is scant (see for an exception). It is currently unclear whether U.S. Latino or Middle-Eastern people are seen through a similar masculine–feminine lens; a question we explored here. Given that threat and danger perceptions of U.S. Latino and Middle-Eastern individuals can be similar to those of U.S. Black Americans (; ), we expected that they would also be seen as particularly masculine.

Study 1: Normative Stereotypes at the Intersection of Gender and Sexual Orientation

Method

Participants

We used a rule-of-thumb strategy to recruit roughly 100 participants per condition (). We recruited participants from the Harvard Digital Lab for the Social Sciences (DLABSS; www.dlabss.harvard.edu; ), a growing pool of over 16,000 people who agree to participate in research on a volunteer basis. Volunteers complete a master survey and complete as many studies available on the website as they wish. Participants in the pool are primarily based in the U.S. with the northeast region overrepresented. We only recruited participants who lived in the U.S. We did not include direct attention checks to assess the quality of the data, which is a limitation. DLABSS participants volunteered their time and were not paid for their participation. Of the 560 recruited participants, 309 finished the entire study with an average completion time of 24 min. The only exclusion criterion we employed was the failure to finish the study. Participants in Study 1 predominantly self-identified as White (n = 244), with the rest of the population identifying as Black (n = 12), Asian (n = 9), American Indian (n = 2), and “Other” (n = 14). Twenty-eight people did not indicate their racial group. Participants also predominantly self-identified as heterosexual (n = 256) while 19 identified as gay or lesbian, 15 as bisexual, four as pansexual, four as asexual, three as queer, two as questioning, three as “other”, and three preferring not to answer. Finally, 129 participants self-identified as male, 177 as female, two as “other” and one preferring not to answer (In both studies, we asked participants for their gender but mistakenly included labels for sex; for further breakdown of participant demographics, see the online Supplemental materials Tables S1 and S2).

Materials and Procedure

We asked participants to indicate the desirability of three targets displaying a series of 70 traits such as being “Assertive,” “Warm and kind,” and “Honest” (see online Supplemental materials for the full list of traits). These traits were amalgamated from several sources to encompass traits that are historically considered masculine, feminine, and gender-neutral (; ; ), as well as traits that are often used to describe sexually minoritized groups (e.g., “Theatrical” for gay people) or racially minoritized groups (e.g., “Musical” for U.S. Black Americans). Participants first familiarized themselves with the traits before moving to the three trait rating tasks.

All participants completed three rating tasks—one for a man, one for a woman, and one for a person—for a total of 210 traits rated on desirability. What differed between participants was the sexual orientation of the man, woman, and person targets they rated. We randomly assigned participants to rate trait desirability for straight targets (i.e., a straight man, straight woman, and straight person), gay targets (i.e., a gay man, lesbian woman, and homosexual person), or label-less targets (i.e., a man, a woman, and person). The label-less condition represented the standard way of assessing gender normative stereotypes (subsequently referred to as the “Control condition”) and constituted a direct replication of for 60 of the 70 traits used. We note that the use of “homosexual” to denote the gender-neutral person term in the gay condition is now considered offensive and does not conform to ; ) guidelines for inclusive language. We recommend future work use more inclusive language such as a sexually minoritized person.

Participants rated the traits on a 9-point scale from 1 (extremely undesirable) to 9 (extremely desirable) while answering, “How desirable is it in American society for a [TARGET] to possess each of the following characteristics?” We purposefully asked participants to indicate how they thought the “average American” would respond to help reduce demand characteristics and social desirability concerns (). We recognize that the use of “American” to refer to people from the U.S. was unnecessarily U.S.-centric. We do believe that participants understood what geographic area we were limiting their responses to, as all participants were currently living in the U.S.

The targets were presented sequentially such that participants rated trait desirability for all 70 traits for one target before moving on to the next. The order of the man and woman targets was displayed in a counterbalanced order such that the man target was first or second compared to the woman target, while the person target was always rated last. After the three rating tasks, participants described what they thought a typical straight and gay man as well as a typical straight or lesbian woman looked and acted like in a free-response task. Finally, participants filled out a series of exploratory questionnaires, including a sexual prototypicality scale and the Internal and External Motivation to appear Non-Prejudiced scale (IMS-EMS; ) adapted for LGBTQ people. We do not discuss the free responses or the exploratory questionnaires in this paper.

Data Analysis Strategy

We analyzed the data using a multilevel regression model in which Target Gender (3 Levels: Man, Woman, Person), Target Sexual Orientation (3 Levels: Straight, Gay/Lesbian, Control), and Traits (70 Levels: Aggressive, Ambitious, Approval Seeking, etc.) interacted to predict desirability ratings, as well as included a participant-level random intercept to account for the within-subject Trait and Target Gender factors. Target Gender and Trait were within-subjects while Target Sexual Orientation was between-subjects. We effects-coded each of our variables with “Person,” the Control condition, and the trait “Yielding” as reference categories. We also accounted for the impact of Participant Gender on desirability ratings as done in previous research () by including the two-way interactions between Participant Gender (effects coded) and each of our variables of interest. This was important as women overall reported more extreme stereotype prescriptions and proscriptions than men.

There were two alternative data analytic approaches we considered, namely running individual models for each trait as well as reducing the number of traits considered through some form of aggregation. We found these alternative approaches to be less optimal than the one described above. First, sequentially running 70 (for each trait) with in-between analyses of variance (ANOVAs) could not take into consideration the fact that each trait rating within Target Gender (210 trait ratings in total) was related. Thus, using multilevel models, while complex, allows for the greatest accounting of within-subject variance. Second, we did not reduce the number of trait categories for two reasons. The first reason was that these studies are meant to showcase the landscape of gender normative stereotypes at the intersection of sexual orientation and gender, allowing the current work to serve as a useful point of reference for future work on specific attributes that center intersectionality. Leaving the traits disaggregated fully showcases the richness of normative stereotypes.

The second reason was methodological in that reducing the data in either a bottom-up or top-down approach had problems. Creating superordinate categories from theory (i.e., top–down) requires us to make assumptions about the nature of gender stereotypes that the current work is meant to challenge. For example, what traits should be labeled masculine or feminine is likely group-specific, making any aggregation a poorer fit for one group relative to another. Similarly, creating superordinate categories in a bottom-up approach (e.g., exploratory factor analysis) makes it impossible to compare specific stereotypes across groups, one of the main contributions to the literature of the current design in conjunction with others, such as open-ended free responses (). In the spirit of serving as a reference, all data, data analysis scripts, descriptive statistics, and Supplementary analyses for this study as well as Study 2, can be found on the Center for Open Science website (https://osf.io/nu8v2/).

We used the resulting models as the basis for planned comparisons across and within Target Gender, Target Sexual Orientation, and Traits. To account for Type 1 error, we used the “emmeans” package in R (), controlling for multiple comparisons for all contrasts using the more stringent Bonferroni method. For large samples and complicated models, “emmeans” uses z-statistics for hypothesis testing instead of t-statistics, as it is much easier to calculate computationally. In this instance, using a z-test is the equivalent of using a t-test with infinite degrees of freedom. As the t-distribution approximates the z-distribution (i.e., the standard normal distribution) above 30 degrees of freedom, this is a reasonable substitution given our sample size in each factor-combination. At each stage, the more complicated model better fit the data as the marginal pseudo-R2 () was .50.

Using this analytic strategy, we calculated the study's sensitivity to detect an effect as small as d = 0.04 (f2 = 0.02), assuming the ANOVA repeated measures within-between interaction setting in G*Power (), an alpha level of 0.95, 80% power, three between-subject groups, and 210 repeated measures. Given our posthoc tests, a more relevant sensitivity analysis showed we could detect a t-test effect as small as 0.13–0.41, which includes the range for the potential paired and between-subject t-tests.

Results

Table 1 contains the average desirability ratings for each trait by Target Gender and Target Sexual Orientation, classified as either prescriptive, proscriptive, or neutral (the ANOVA tables for all interactions for desirability ratings and desirability difference scores can be found in the online Supplemental Materials, Tables S3 and S4, respectively). More specifically, a prescribed trait was one in which the desirability point-estimate for the target was significantly higher than the midpoint (i.e., 5) while a proscribed trait was one in which the point-estimate was significantly below the midpoint. Neutral traits were those for which the point estimate was not significantly different from the midpoint (i.e., the 99% confidence interval [CI] included “5”).

Hypothesis 1: Gender Normative Stereotypes Still Exist

We hypothesized that the gender normative stereotypes uncovered by Prentice and Carranza in 2002 would still exist in the U.S. We investigated this question by examining gender differences in trait desirability between “Man” and “Woman” in the Control condition. As hypothesized, women and men were held to different normative standards on a variety of traits (see online Supplemental materials, Table S5 for comparisons with findings from ). There were 55 traits out of 70 (78.6%) for which participants’ desirability for a man versus a woman to display those traits significantly differed. The trait differences in desirability for a man versus a woman were robust, with some trait differences (e.g., feminine and masculine) as large as six units on a nine-point scale. Thus, Hypothesis 1 was supported.

The top five prescriptive stereotypes for men included being self-reliant (M= 8.17), displaying leadership ability (M= 8.09), being ambitious (M= 8.06), being masculine (M= 8.03), and being decisive (M= 8.03), and the top five prescriptive stereotypes for women included being feminine (M= 8.08), being warm and kind (M= 8.01), being patient (M= 7.86), paying attention to appearances (M= 7.77), and being polite (M= 7.74). The top five proscriptive stereotypes for men included being weak (M= 1.77), being feminine (M= 2.16), being naïve (M= 2.16), being gullible (M= 2.17), and being child-like (M= 2.45), and the top five for women were proscribed the most from being arrogant, (M= 2.16), being masculine (M= 2.28), being controlling (M= 2.68), being promiscuous (M= 3.04), and being aggressive (M= 3.15; see Table S6 in the online Supplemental Materials for the full listing of trait desirabilities by target. Table S7 contains the prescriptive, proscriptive, and neutral designations of traits in general while Tables S8–S13 contain these labels by target).

Hypothesis 2: Androcentric and Heterocentric Biases Impact Perceptions of Normative Stereotypes

Next, we examined whether people's normative stereotypes were influenced by androcentrism and heterocentrism.

Androcentrism

We empirically defined androcentrism to mean that trait desirabilities would be more similar, or have greater conceptual overlap, between “Person” and “Man” targets compared to “Person” and “Woman” targets. We examined androcentrism by looking at the interaction between Target Gender and Trait, finding a significant interaction, F(138, 62299.76) = 40.06, p < .001. Collapsing across sexual orientation, the data supported an androcentric bias in gender normative stereotypes (Table 2), supporting Hypothesis 2 (analyses that also included Target Sexual Orientation were explored in Hypothesis 3). Overall, there were 30 traits for which the Person categories differed from the “Man” categories. In contrast, there were 42 traits for which the “Person” categories differed from the “Woman” categories. Of the 30 traits for which the Man categories differed from the Person categories, only four of them were traits for which the “Woman” categories did not also differ from the “Person” categories (i.e., Cooperative, Cynical, Expresses emotion, and Stubborn).

Heterocentrism

We empirically defined heterocentrism to mean that the trait desirabilities for targets without a specified sexual orientation—“Man,” “Woman,” and “Person”—would be more similar to “Straight Man,” “Straight Woman,” and “Straight Person,” respectively, than “Gay Man,” “Lesbian Woman,” and “Homosexual Person.” We examined heterocentrism by comparing Target Sexual Orientation across traits, collapsing across Target Gender (see Hypothesis 3 for the three-way interaction). In support of Hypothesis 2, there was a significant interaction between Target Sexual Orientation and Trait, F(138, 62299.68) = 34.91, p < .001 (Figure 1). There was only a single trait for which the “Control” targets differed from the “Straight” targets (i.e., Materialistic). In contrast, there were 63 traits for which the “Control” targets significantly differed from the “Gay” targets (see Table 2).

Open multimedia modal
Figure 1
Open multimedia modal

Heterocentrism in Gender Stereotypes.

Note. Each point represents the average desirability rating for a single trait by target sexual orientation (represented by dot color/shape) and by target sex (showcased in different panels). Black open-circles represent the control condition; Blue squares represent the straight condition, and pink open-triangles represent the gay/lesbian condition. Higher numbers represent greater average desirability for a given trait; traits below the midpoint (5) are proscribed and traits above the midpoint are prescribed. Heterocentrism is represented by how similar the black circles are to the blue squares relative to how similar the black circles are to the pink triangles.

Hypothesis 3: Sexual Orientation and Race Altered the Prescriptive Stereotypes of Men and Women

We hypothesized that we would see evidence of gender inversion in normative stereotypes such that the normative stereotypes of gay men would be similar to straight women, while the normative stereotypes of lesbian women would be similar to straight men. We labeled traits as showing evidence of gender inversion if there were significant gender differences between straight men and straight women, as well as between gay men and lesbian women. Furthermore, the direction of the gender difference needed to be reversed from one sexual orientation to another—for example, if it was more desirable for a straight man to be aggressive compared to a straight woman, aggressiveness needed to be less desirable for a gay man compared to a lesbian woman. As expected, the three-way interaction was significant, F(276, 62299.72) = 17.69, p < .001. All pairwise comparisons for each trait by Target Gender and Target Sexual Orientation can be found in the online Supplemental materials (Table S14), as well as graphs of each trait plotted separately (see online Supplemental Materials in Figure S4).

Gender Inversion

There were 15 out of 70 traits for which there was gender inversion (Figure 2 and Figure S1). A subset of these traits (i.e., Attention to appearances, Cheerful, Excitable, Expresses emotion, Friendly, Stubborn, Theatrical, and Warm and kind) showed a unique type of gender inversion in which the desirability ratings for either the man or the woman target was the same for all sexual orientations but diverged for the opposite gender. What made this pattern of gender inversion noteworthy was that the normative pressure differed by sexual orientation for only one gender. For example, the desirability of paying attention to appearances showed gender inversion such that straight women and gay men faced a stronger prescription to pay attention to their appearances compared to straight men and lesbian women, respectively. However, the desirability of paying attention to appearances was the same for men of all sexual orientations but diverged for women, with people stating a lowered desirability for lesbian women to pay attention to their appearances compared to straight women. “Excitable,” “Expresses emotion,” and “Theatrical,” were the three traits for which there was normative pressure only on men of different sexual orientations compared to women.

Open multimedia modal
Figure 2
Open multimedia modal

Gender Differences in Normative Stereotypes for Gay and Straight Targets.

Note. Each dot represents the average gender difference between straight men and straight women (blue circles) as well as between gay men and lesbian women (pink circles) for a given trait. If the circle is filled with black, the gender difference for that trait was significantly greater than zero; if the circle is filled with white, the gender difference was not significantly different from zero. Circles above zero represent traits for which the man target had a greater desirability rating than the woman target; circles below zero represent traits for which the woman target had a greater desirability rating than the man target.

Other Interaction Patterns

Gender inversion did not describe the pattern of prescription and proscription for the bulk of the traits, however. Instead, for 41 traits, we found a pattern we are labeling as “Sexual Orientation Asymmetry,” such that there were differences in the normative expectations between a man and a woman, but only for one of the sexual orientations (see Figures S2 and S3). For 37 traits, there were significant gender differences between the straight targets but not for the gay/lesbian targets (i.e., Ambitious, Arrogant, Athletic, Business sense, Career oriented, Child-like, Competitive, Cooperative, Cynical, Decisive/able to make decisions, Defends own beliefs, Emotional, Feminine, Gullible, Happy, High self-esteem, Impressionable, Intelligent, Leadership ability, Loves children, Masculine, Melodramatic, Naïve, Patient, Polite, Promiscuous, Rational, Rebellious, Self-reliant, Shy, Spiritual, Strong personality, Weak, Well-educated, Wholesome, Willing to take risks, and Yielding). There were only four traits for which there were significant differences between gay men and lesbian women but not between straight men and women (i.e., Flirtatious, Materialistic, Musical, and Well-dressed). Thus, people had distinct gendered norms for how straight men and straight women should and should not act but did not have as strong gendered expectations for gay men and lesbian women. This conclusion is further supported by the fact that there were 52 traits for which trait desirability for a straight man differed from a straight woman but only 19 traits for which trait desirability differed between a gay man and a lesbian woman.

The last 14 traits did not show any gender differentiation in either straight or gay/lesbian targets. Of these traits, 13 of them were traits that showed a main effect of Target Sexual Orientation. More specifically, trait desirability was higher for straight targets than gay/lesbian targets for 10 traits (i.e., Clean, Conservative, Consistent, Dependable, Disciplined, Enthusiastic, Honest, Likeable, Loyal, and Protects others) while trait desirability was higher for gay targets than straight targets for three traits (i.e., Moody, Nosy, and Superstitious).

Study 2: Normative Stereotypes at the Intersection of Gender and Race

Method

Participants

We again recruited roughly 100 participants per condition. Participants in this study came from Amazon Mechanical Turk (MTurk; www.mturk.com) recruited by TurkPrime (now CloudResearch, www.cloudresearch.com). MTurk is an online platform where people can complete tasks as a function of their skill sets and demographic characteristics for pay. MTurk is convenient and allows for easy recruitment of large sample sizes within a short amount of time. Research has found MTurk participants to be more diverse than participants from standard online pools and U.S. colleges (; ), although there can be concerns about low data quality (). Unfortunately, we again did not include direct attention checks.

We restricted the pool of participants to those local to the U. S. We paid our MTurk participants $1.20 for their time on a survey that took an average of 23 min to complete. Six hundred and nineteen out of 694 participants finished the study in full and were included in the analyses. Participants predominantly self-identified as White (n = 429), with the rest of the participants self-identifying as Black (n = 71), East Asian (n = 35), South Asian (n = 32), multiracial (n = 31), Latino/a (n = 8), Middle-Eastern (n = 6), and “Other” (n = 4). Three people declined to indicate their racial group. More people identified as female(n = 326) than as male(n = 284), while three participants indicated they were transgender. Six participants declined to give any information about their sex (for further information on sample demographic characteristics, see online Supplemental materials, Table S15). Due to a coding error, we did not collect sexual orientation information in this study.

Materials and Procedure

The procedure for Study 2 was identical to Study 1 with the sole change of Target Race replacing Target Sexual Orientation. Participants rated the desirability of the same 70 traits for targets that differed by gender (within-subjects) and race (between-subjects). We randomly assigned participants to one of six conditions: rating trait desirability for a man, a woman, and a person that was either Asian, Black, Latino, Middle-Eastern, White, or label-less (the “Control condition”). After the three rating tasks, participants described what they thought a typical man or woman of different races looked and acted like in a free-response task. Finally, participants filled out a series of exploratory questionnaires, including a racial prototypicality scale and the IMS-EMS scale () adapted for ethnic minorities. We do not discuss the free-responses or the exploratory questionnaires in this paper.

Data Analysis Strategy

We analyzed the data in Study 2 similarly to Study 1, but instead of Target Sexual Orientation, we interacted Target Race (six levels: White, Black, Asian, Latino, Middle-Eastern, Control) with Target Gender and Traits predicting desirability ratings using a multilevel model, including participant random intercept and the two-way interactions with Participant Gender. Target Gender and Trait were again within-subjects while Target Race was between-subjects. At each stage, the more complicated model better fit the data, as the marginal pseudo-R2 () in Study 2 was .33. Table 3 (as well as Table S18 in the online Supplementary materials) contains the average desirability ratings for each trait by Target Gender and Target Race, classified as either prescriptive, proscriptive, or neutral (the ANOVA Tables for all interactions for desirability ratings and desirability difference scores can be found in the online Supplemental materials, Tables S16 and S17, respectively).

Results

Hypothesis 1: Gender Normative Stereotypes Still Exist

Men and women were also held to different normative standards in the Control condition within Study 2. There were 48 traits out of 70 (68.6%) for which participants’ desirability for a man versus a woman to display those traits significantly differed. To place the continuity of gendered prescriptions in context, there were 60 traits that both we and studied. Of those 60, Prentice and Carranza found significant gender differences in 55 of them. We found significant differences for 47 of the 60 in Study 1 and 41 in Study 2, supporting Hypothesis 1 (see online Supplemental Table S5). The continuity of gender normative stereotypes extended to the more nuanced categorization of traits as intensified and relaxed prescriptive and proscriptive as outlined in . Those analyses can be found in the online Supplemental materials.

The most prescribed and proscribed stereotypes for men and women showed remarkable consistency across Study 1 and Study 2, as the top five prescriptive stereotypes for men again included being self-reliant (M = 7.85), displaying leadership ability (M = 7.93), being ambitious (M = 7.77), being masculine (M = 7.99), and being decisive (M = 7.78). The top five prescriptive stereotypes for women included being feminine (M = 8.00), being warm and kind (M = 7.73), being patient (M = 7.59), paying attention to appearances (M = 7.83), and being polite (M = 7.86). The top five proscriptive stereotypes for men included being weak (M = 2.15), being feminine (M = 2.34), naïve (M = 2.66), being gullible (M = 2.44), and being child-like (M = 2.78), and women were proscribed the most from being arrogant, (M = 2.71), being masculine (M = 2.36), controlling (M = 3.05), promiscuous (M = 3.54), and aggressive (M = 3.11; see Table S18 in the online Supplemental materials for the full listing of traits by target. Tables S19–S20 contain the traits labeled as prescriptive, proscriptive, and neutral overall, while Tables S21–S32 contain the traits labeled as intensified and relaxed by target.).

Hypothesis 2: Cognitive biases Impact People's Normative Stereotypes

Androcentrism

We found a significant interaction between Trait and Target Gender, F(138, 127728.01) = 48.61, p < .001 such that there were more traits for which “Person” differed from “Woman” than traits for which “Person” differed from “Men” (the three-way interaction is analyzed in Hypothesis 3). Evidence for androcentrism was not as strong as it was in Study 1 but still present (Table 2; see online Supplemental materials Figures S5–S9), as there were 37 traits for which the “Person” categories differed from the “Man” categories but 42 traits for which the “Person” categories differed from the “Woman” categories. Of the 37 traits for which the “Man” categories differed from the “Person” categories, 10 of them were traits for which the “Woman” categories did not also differ from the “Person” categories (i.e., Attention to appearances, Cheerful, Happy, Patient, Polite, Promiscuous, Rebellious, Spiritual, Stubborn, and Wholesome).

Eurocentrism

We empirically defined Eurocentrism to mean the trait desirabilities for targets without a specified race—“Man,” “Woman,” and “Person”—would be more similar to “White Man,” “White Woman,” and “White Person,” respectively compared to the “Man,” “Woman,” and “Person” targets of any other racial group. There was a significant interaction between Target Race and Trait, F(345, 127728.01) = 8.77, p < .001 but limited evidence of Eurocentrism (Table 2). Although there was the fewest number of traits for which the “Control” targets differed from the “White” targets (n = 19), it was only one less than the number of traits for “Black” targets (n = 20). The level of overlap between the “Control” and “Asian” targets was the same for “Black” targets, but for decidedly different stereotypes as only six stereotypes were in both sets (i.e., Assertive, Defends own beliefs, Flirtatious, High self-esteem, Leadership ability, and Protects others). There was comparatively less overlap between the “Control” and “Middle-Eastern” targets (n = 29) as well as between “Control” and “Latino” targets (n = 31). Thus, Hypothesis 2 was supported in terms of androcentrism but not supported regarding Eurocentrism.

Hypothesis 3: Race Altered the Perceived Normativity of Stereotypes Between Men and Women

Although there was not strong evidence for Eurocentrism in stereotypes in isolation, gendered stereotypes as a dynamic between men and women showed evidence of Eurocentrism (for additional details, see online Supplemental materials Figures S5–S10). As expected, the three-way interaction was significant, F(690, 127728.01) = 3.18, p < .001. We hypothesized normative stereotypes would conform to expectations set by the gendered race hypothesis: gender normative stereotypes for Black men and women would be masculinized, while normative stereotypes for Asian men and women would be feminized (). We also explored whether we would find similar patterns for Latino and Middle Eastern men and women. We tested our hypotheses around the gendered race hypothesis by first examining racial differences in the desirability to be masculine and feminine (See online Supplemental materials Tables S33–S35 for all pairwise comparisons and Figure S15 for each trait graphed individually.).

Gendered Race

First examining masculinity, the gendered race hypothesis predicted that Black men would have the highest norms to be masculine, followed by White men, and then Asian men. Contrary to hypotheses, it was most desirable for a White man (M = 7.94) to be masculine compared to a Black (M = 6.76), Asian (M = 6.14), Latino (M = 6.94), and Middle-Eastern (M = 6.67) man. All ethnic minority men in contrast were held to the same standards around masculinity as each other, ps > .888. In contrast, there were no differences in the desirability of White (M = 3.06), Black (M = 3.14), Asian (M = 3.11), Latino (M = 3.28), or Middle-Eastern (M = 3.18) women to be masculine, ps > .999. This result is also contrary to the gendered race hypothesis, which presumes that Black women specifically would be desired to be more masculine compared to White and Asian women (See online materials Figure S10).

Next, we looked at the desirability to be feminine. Here there was some support for the gendered race hypothesis. Although all men were proscribed from being feminine, it was more desirable for an Asian man (M = 3.97) to be feminine compared to a White man (M = 2.84), who did not differ from Black (M = 2.96), Latino (M = 3.39), or Middle-Eastern (M = 3.00) men. It was also less desirable for a Black (M = 6.67) and a Middle-Eastern (M = 6.68) woman to be feminine compared to a White woman (M = 7.86), who did not differ from an Asian (M = 7.23) or Latina (M = 7.11) woman.

To continue to investigate the masculinity or femininity of racial gender norms, we assessed, on a trait-by-trait basis, whether people's normative stereotypes for “Man” and “Woman” (i.e., normative stereotypes in the control condition) were similar to, or different than, the stereotypes of men and women of different races. To do this, we tabulated whether the 99% confidence intervals [CIs] for each trait desirability for Asian, Black, Latino, Middle-Eastern, and White men and women included the desirability point estimate for a “man” and “woman” respectively. We then compared whether the normative stereotypes of men and women of different races had greater, or less, overlap with people's normative expectations for men overall, suggesting hyper- or hypo-masculinization, respectively. We did the same analyses for hyper- and hypo-femininization as well.

First examining the men targets, overall, White men were hyper-masculinized compared to men of other races, χ (4, N = 70) = 24.20, p < .001, as there were significantly more traits for which the White man's 99% CIs included the control condition “Man” point estimate compared to Black men, Asian Men, Latino men, and Middle-Eastern men. In contrast, there was no difference between men of different races in the degree of overlap for the control condition “Woman” point estimates, χ (4, N = 70) = 2.54, p = .638. Regarding women, overall Asian women were hyper-feminized and Black women were hypo-feminized compared to White, Latina, and Middle-Eastern women, χ (4, N = 70) = 21.48, p < .001, supporting the gendered race hypothesis, and there was no difference between women of different races in the degree of overlap for the control condition “Man” point estimates, χ (4, N = 70) = 5.85, p = .211.

Other Interaction Patterns

Examining the traits overall, as was the case for sexual orientation, there was more pronounced gender differentiation in stereotypes for the majoritized White targets than for the racially minoritized groups: A pattern of “Race Asymmetry” (See online Supplemental materials Figures S11–S14) More specifically, close to half of the traits (n = 34) showed significant gender differences between a White man and a White woman (i.e., Aggressive, Ambitious, Approval seeking, Arrogant, Assertive, Athletic, Business sense, Career oriented, Compassionate, Competitive, Controlling, Decisive/able to make decisions, Defends own beliefs, Emotional, Expresses emotion, Feminine, Forceful, Gullible, High self-esteem, Leadership ability, Loves children, Masculine, Naïve, Protects others, Rebellious, Self-reliant, Sensitive, Shy, Strong personality, Stubborn, Warm and kind, Weak, Willing to take risks, and Yielding). In contrast, none of the other racial groups had more than a fourth of the traits showing the same level of gender differentiation. There were 16 traits for which this was true for Middle-Eastern targets (i.e., Aggressive, Ambitious, Assertive, Business sense, Career oriented, Competitive, Controlling, Decisive/able to make decisions, Feminine, Forceful, Leadership ability, Masculine, Naïve, Sensitive, Shy, and Strong personality), 12 traits for Asian targets (i.e., Aggressive, Business sense, Career oriented, Child-like, Competitive, Expresses emotion, Feminine, Leadership ability, Masculine, Naïve, Sensitive, and Shy), four traits for Latino targets (i.e., Athletic, Feminine, Masculine, and Sensitive), and only three traits for Black targets (i.e., Athletic, Feminine, and Masculine).

Finally, of the 36 traits that did not show gender differences in White targets, 25 of them showed a main effect of Race for Black targets, 25 for Middle-Eastern targets, 22 for Latino targets, and 15 for Asian targets. For almost all these stereotypes where there was a main effect of Race, there was a greater desire for White targets to display them than for racial minorities.

General Discussion

There is a large body of work on the nature and impact of gender-normative stereotypes. However, this research has predominantly been done in isolation of other identities, such as sexual orientation and race, that likely impact the normative expectations men and women are under. In the present work, we explored the landscape of gender normative stereotypes by asking people to rate the desirability of a man, woman, and person of various sexual orientations in Study 1, and races in Study 2, to display 70 traits, from the perspective of an average American. Overall, we found support for most of our hypotheses. As expected, gender normative stereotypes are still perceived to be pervasive forces in society, assuming the perspective of an average American. Across 70 traits, about 70% of them showed robust gender differences.

Furthermore, we found support for the presence of two out of three cognitive biases. There was robust evidence of heterocentrism, as people's normative expectations for men and women conformed to the expectations of straight men and women rather than gay men and lesbian women. Normative stereotypes showed consistent evidence of androcentrism, as people's trait desirabilities for “Person” categories were closer to “Man” than “Woman” categories. However, normative stereotypes did not support Eurocentrism, as the patterns seen in the Control condition did not conform more to White targets than other racially minoritized groups. While there was not strong evidence of Eurocentrism on the level of Trait, there was Eurocentrism in the normative pressures applied to men and women as a dyadic unit; the stereotypes of how men and women should and should not behave in relation to one another best-matched people's normative stereotypes of White men and women compared to any other racial group.

Finally, there was some support for the gender inversion theory in Study 1 and the gendered race hypothesis in Study 2. In Study 1, of the 52 traits that showed gender differences between straight men and straight women, 15 of them also showed an inverted pattern of gendered differences between gay men and lesbian women. In Study 2, contrary to our expectation that Black men and women would be relatively more masculinized, White men were hyper-masculinized. Asian men and women, as expected, were relatively more feminized, with Latino and Middle-Eastern men and women seen similarly to White men and women in terms of femininity and masculinity. However, across both studies and for the bulk of the traits, participants did not draw large distinctions between the normative expectations of minoritized men and women. Indeed, participants reported significant differences in their desirability of a Black man compared to a Black woman to display certain traits for only three out of 70 traits. Given that Target Gender was a within-subject factor, we set up the best-case situation to allow for differentiation by gender through contrast effects, and yet we only found it for the majoritized groups.

In sum, from a methodological standpoint, gendered targets without an explicitly labeled social identity were not seen neutrally. This suggests that research that does not explicitly specify social identities likely represents the majoritized view of majoritized groups to an outsized degree, impacting the generalizability of such work, and supporting our theoretical critique.

Implications

These findings have several implications for the work on gender normative stereotypes specifically, and gender research more broadly. We highlight two. The first is that our current understanding of gender normative stereotypes (limited as it may be to the perspectives of dominant group members) might only apply to minoritized groups under restricted situations. Across both sexual orientation and race, being part of a minoritized group had a greater influence on people's normative expectations than being a particular gender. People did not desire gay/lesbian, Black, Asian, Latino, and Middle-Eastern men and women to act as differently from one another as they desired straight and White men and women to act. This implies that the normative pressures on minoritized groups fluctuate depending upon which aspect of their identity is foregrounded. Research on representations of transgender men and women has found a similar lack of gender differentiation within minoritized targets (), a phenomenon they label as “de-gendering.” Other work has also found that global stereotypes around competence and warmth for Black men and women show more similarity than the same stereotypes between White men and women (), further supporting the conclusion that gender differentiation is reduced within minoritized groups.

Interestingly, within racially minoritized groups, we found that people differentiated Middle-Eastern men from Middle-Eastern women the most. We can only speculate, but it could be due to beliefs that Middle-Eastern people are particularly religious and conservative (), and thus this group retains normative expectations in line with traditional gender roles. It could also be due to weaker group stereotypes, and thus participants defaulted to more prototypical gender stereotypes, which reflect majoritized groups. Finally, there could be an effect of status, as perceptions of race and status are related (; , ). For example, U.S. Middle-Eastern and Asian Americans are stereotyped as rich while U.S. Black and Latino Americans are stereotyped as poor (); we found the greatest gender differences in the targets that are stereotyped as wealthier. Indeed, social role theory posits that the stereotypes of men and women come from the social roles they occupy (). Gender is not the only social identity that has correlations with expected social roles along class and status dimensions, supporting an intersectional approach to normative stereotypes. More research is needed on the normative expectations of minoritized groups more broadly to understand when individuals default to stereotypes about one identity over another ().

Second, while there is often concordance between descriptive and normative stereotypes () for men and women, that concordance became untethered in minoritized groups. In other words, for majoritized straight and White individuals, descriptive stereotypes match normative stereotypes. How people assume society expects straight men and women to be matches how straight men and women ought to be and ought not to be as well. However, there seems to be a mismatch for minoritized targets. One glaring example is the masculinity associated with White and Black men. Descriptively Black men are seen as more masculine than White men (; ). If the descriptive stereotypes associated with Black men in terms of masculinity matched normative stereotypes, we would expect participants to also indicate that it is most desirable for Black men (out of other races) to be masculine. Instead, we found that people desired Black men to be less masculine than White men.

This finding suggests that if a Black man displays the same level of masculinity as a White man, he is already too masculine and thus might face penalties for such behavior when compared to White men. Given the large amount of research on discrimination faced by Black men because of stereotypes around dangerousness and threat (; ; ), comparing descriptive and normative expectations of Black men gives us another account of how these disparities can arise. Indeed, the discrepancy between descriptive and normative stereotypes also can explain why Black men who act communally or appear non-threatening are rewarded (; ).

Normative stereotypes associated with lesbian women also show this discrepancy between descriptive and normative stereotypes. Work on descriptive stereotypes broadly shows that people assume lesbian women are like straight men across a multitude of traits and characteristics. Normatively, however, there seems to be a distinct split in the masculine traits that lesbian women are desired to embody more so than gay men or straight women compared to the ones where they are not. For example, although it was more desirable for lesbians to be “Aggressive,” “Assertive,” and “Forceful” compared to gay men or straight women, there were no differences in the desirability for traits like possessing “Business sense,” being “Career oriented,” or having “Leadership abilities.” This suggests that although it might be acceptable for lesbian women to possess more masculine traits from a personality perspective, it is not always acceptable for them to possess masculine traits in ways that would disturb the gender status quo. This split in masculine traits is mirrored in work on descriptive stereotypes of women of different races. Black women, who are also seen as more masculine, are seen as agentic from a dominance perspective but not from a competence perspective ().

Limitations

While this study expands our understanding of gender normative stereotypes, there are limitations to the research presented here. The most obvious limitation is the lack of diversity within our samples, especially regarding sexual orientation and race. We extended work on gender normative stereotypes by disaggregating the categories of “men,” “women,” and “people,” by sexual orientation and race; we did not want to further complicate the design by including participant sexual orientation and race into the analyses as well. Given that we asked participants to indicate the beliefs of “an average American,” the same cognitive biases studied here likely influenced participants’ answers, making it even more probable that these findings represent the beliefs of White and straight members of the U.S. Thus, we want to be very explicit and say that these findings represent the normative beliefs of majoritized U.S. White, straight group members, and should not be assumed to generalize beyond those groups. That being said, understanding the normative pressures majoritized groups place on others is important as these individuals disproportionately hold political, economic, and social power. Consequently, their normative beliefs are more likely to influence real-world outcomes like backlash and discrimination. Phenomena such as third-party prejudice are predicated upon the pluralistic beliefs embedded within society (). Future studies should investigate the normative pressures minoritized groups place on themselves rather than expectations of what the broader society desires.

Another limitation is our simplified understanding of gender and sexual orientation. We assumed a gender binary in our design, asking individuals about their stereotypes of men and women. This design cannot further our understanding of genderfluid, genderqueer, or non-binary individuals; future studies should examine normative stereotypes for individuals along the gender spectrum. Similarly, we only assessed stereotypes for gay men and lesbian women without acknowledging the diversity within those categories as well as other sexual and gender minorities like bisexual or transgender men and women, respectively. Participants likely assumed a more feminine gay man and a more masculine lesbian woman, but we do not have evidence to speak to this. The fact that multiple subtypes of gay men (e.g., body-conscious gay men or flamboyant gay men) and lesbian women (e.g., butch lesbian, feminine lesbian) exist (; ) might be driving some of the patterns observed. We did not find a strong norm for gay men or lesbian women to be masculine or feminine, which could be due to participants aggregating across the subtypes. Additional intersectional work is needed to look at subtypes within these social categories.

A third limitation is that we assessed stereotypes of people of different races without specifying other social identities that likely were assumed. For example, being a Muslim is often associated with individuals identifying as Middle-Eastern, thus, stereotypes towards U.S. Middle-Eastern Americans likely reflect, to some extent, beliefs about U.S. Muslim Americans as well. As another example, we asked about U.S. Asian Americans, but the prototype for U.S. Asian Americans is East Asian rather than South Asian (; ; ). Finally, there has been a recent surge of stereotype and perception research at the intersection of race and sexual orientation (; ; ; ), suggesting that these categories also influence one another in ways we do not examine here. Thus, the lack of effects by gender and race could be due to divergent representations of subgroups that we did not account for.

A fourth limitation is in the unidimensional way we approach the selected traits. These dynamics can become further complicated because how one is compelled to perform “warmth” or “intelligence” might change at the intersection. For example, while we found that Black women faced reduced prescriptions to be “feminine” compared to White women, how femininity is understood and reinforced is likely qualitatively different for White and Black women. We did not give definitions of any of our traits, meaning that more precise definitions could yield different results. As another example, both White women and women of color have stereotypes related to being sexual, but only Black and Latina women are described as “promiscuous” (). White women are “sexually liberal,” suggesting that people might represent the trait “promiscuous” with different connotations that matter for backlash and discrimination.

Finally, our determinations of gendered patterns relied on statistical breaks. We struck a balance between statistical rigor and not letting a focus on p-values overshadow the exploratory nature of the work. However, we let the data tell us where the important cleavages in patterns of gendered normative stereotypes lie, and there were some ambiguous cases. For example, while the desirability for straight and gay targets to be clean resulted in a main effect of sexual orientation and not an interaction, the pattern, by eye, is more closely aligned to a gender asymmetry effect. It was more desirable for a straight woman to be cleaner than a lesbian woman but equally as desirable for a straight and gay man to be clean. While this is indeed a gender asymmetry pattern, the fact that there were no significant gender differences within both straight and gay targets precluded it from the category. Thus, we see gender normative stereotypes as more amorphous than what we present, and this is, in part, why it was important to include all means and comparisons in Supplemental materials. We still believe there is substantive utility in our studies, as these studies are but the first of, we hope, many that will examine normative stereotypes intersectionally.

Future Directions

Despite these limitations, the results of these two studies invite inquiry into many avenues of research. We highlight four here. The first is to expand our understanding of intersectional gender normative stereotypes. We believe the work presented is just the beginning of a rich landscape that can parse when certain normative pressures are heightened or mitigated at the intersection of multiple groups. The use of big data might be particularly fruitful in this endeavor. Work using natural language processing has found gendered associations within the broader societal written corpus (). These tools can also be used to assess associations for gender nuanced by race in a more naturalistic setting that is closer to people's lived experiences. What types of words or phrases follow “Black woman” in our written corpus compared to just “woman”? Having answers to this and other questions will further allow for nuanced policy recommendations and perhaps identify areas where the expectations of women differ by subgroups ().

Second, this work encourages further exploration into gendered backlash. The current findings from lesbian and racially minoritized women suggest that how they will experience role violations will be distinct from straight and White women. Although some recent research shows that Black women do not always experience backlash when acting in agentic ways compared to White women and women in general (; ), much of the work blurs over racial distinctions. Third and relatedly, how do people experience contextualized normative pressures? Our work suggests that the normative pressures Black women are under, for example, depend on whether they are being perceived primarily by their race, their gender, or a combination of the two. As people feel compelled to conform to expectations and mitigate negative impressions (; ), there is an outstanding question as to whether intersectional group members accurately assess the norms they are expected to adhere to.

Fourth, these findings also highlight the importance of studying intra-racial and intra-sexual gender dynamics, as the very nature of gender normative stereotypes seems predicated upon the social identities at play. What does it mean for our gendered theories that there is a smaller distinction between the normative stereotypes of gay, Black, Asian, Latino, and Middle Eastern men and women compared to straight and White men and women? How should we understand perceptions of, and engagement in, gender dynamics intra-sexually and intra-racially? We are excited about the new research that will explore these questions.

Practice Implications

Our results have implications for people researching stereotypes at the intersection of identities as well as for those studying disparate treatment based on stereotypes. This work is meant to be a reference for scholars to reflect on how normative expectations might impact intersectional groups in a variety of settings, providing rich soil for hypothesis generation and result analysis. For example, it might be important for a scholar studying the treatment of lesbian women in the workplace to know that lesbian women might be under intensified prescription to be agentic compared to straight women but not necessarily more business oriented. These juxtaposed stereotypes might give their study important context and shape research design. To the extent that the given workplace environment would lead lesbian women to be perceived at the intersection of their gender and sexual orientation, our findings can help foster unique hypotheses that would not be generated with the current understanding utilizing descriptive stereotypes alone. Our work further highlights the importance of explicitly labeling relevant categories that might be tacitly assumed to be a particular subgroup within a category. For example, we have learned to do so ourselves in our misuse of “American” to denote people from the U.S. Such an assumption introduces noise in our data to the extent that the label “American” is unclear to participants and is unnecessarily U.S.-centric. We join the call for work to address the hidden assumptions in researchers’ stimuli and writing.

Declaration of Conflicting Interests The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Funding The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: This research was supported in part by the National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship Program under Grant No. DGE-1745303 as well as the National Science Foundation SBE Postdoctoral Fellowship under Grant No. SBE-2104211 to Hudson, the Harvard Department of Psychology, as well as the Multidisciplinary Inequality and Social Policy and the Women and Public Policy Programs at Harvard Kennedy School. We thank our DLABSS volunteers for their time. We thank Daphne Penn, Brielle Harbin, Jamil Scott, Ryan Lei, and Academic Twitter for their feedback.

ORCID iD Sa-kiera T. J. Hudson https://orcid.org/0000-0002-6607-1704

Supplemental Material Supplement material for this article is available online.

References

  • Alt N. P., Lick D. J., Johnson K. L. (2020). The straight categorization bias: A motivated and altruistic reasoning account. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 119(6), 1266–1289. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000232
    Cited Here
  • American Psychological Association (2015a). Guidelines for psychological practice with transgender and gender nonconforming people. American Psychologist, 70(9), 832–864. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0039906
    Cited Here
  • American Psychological Association (2015b). Key terms and concepts in understanding gender diversity and sexual orientation among students. https://www.apa.org/pi/lgbt/programs/safe-supportive/lgbt/key-terms.pdf
    Cited Here
  • Babbitt L. G. (2013). An intersectional approach to Black/White interracial interactions: The roles of gender and sexual orientation. Sex Roles, 68(11–12), 791–802. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-011-0104-4
    Cited Here
  • Bailey A. H., LaFrance M., Dovidio J. F. (2019). Is man the measure of all things? A social cognitive account of androcentrism. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 23(4), 307–331. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868318782848
    Cited Here
  • Bailey A. H., LaFrance M., Dovidio J. F. (2020). Implicit androcentrism: Men are human, women are gendered. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 89(1), 103980. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2020.103980
    Cited Here
  • Banaji M. R., Hardin C. D. (1996). Automatic stereotyping. Psychological Science, 7(3), 136–141. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1996.tb00346.x
    Cited Here
  • Bartoń K. (2022). MuMIn: Multi-model inference. https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/MuMIn/MuMIn.pdf
    Cited Here
  • Bem S. (1974). The measurement of psychological androgyny. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 42(2), 155–162. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0036215
    Cited Here
  • Berinsky A. J., Huber G. A., Lenz G. S. (2012). Evaluating online labor markets for experimental research: Amazon.com's mechanical turk. Political Analysis, 20(3), 351–368. https://doi.org/10.1093/pan/mpr057
    Cited Here
  • Bhatia N., Bhatia S. (2021). Changes in gender stereotypes over time: A computational analysis. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 45(1), 106–125. https://doi.org/10.1177/0361684320977178
    Cited Here
  • Blashill A. J., Powlishta K. K. (2009a). Gay stereotypes: The use of sexual orientation as a cue for gender-related attributes. Sex Roles, 61(11–12), 783–793. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-009-9684-7
    Cited Here
  • Blashill A. J., Powlishta K. K. (2009b). The impact of sexual orientation and gender role on evaluations of men. Psychology of Men & Masculinity, 10(2), 160–173. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014583
    Cited Here
  • Blashill A. J., Powlishta K. K. (2012). Effects of gender-related domain violations and sexual orientation on perceptions of male and female targets: An analogue study. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 41(5), 1293–1302. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-012-9971-1
    Cited Here
  • Brambilla M., Carnaghi A., Ravenna M. (2011). Status and cooperation shape lesbian stereotypes. Social Psychology, 42(2), 101–110. https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000054
    Cited Here
  • Brescoll V. L., Uhlmann E. L. (2008). Can an angry woman get ahead? Status conferral, gender, and expression of emotion in the workplace. Psychological Science, 19(3), 268–275. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02079.x
    Cited Here
  • Buhrmester M., Kwang T., Gosling S. D. (2011). Amazon’s Mechanical Turk: A new source of inexpensive, yet high-quality, data? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(1), 3–5. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691610393980
    Cited Here
  • Burgess D., Borgida E. (1999). Who women are, who women should be: Descriptive and prescriptive gender stereotyping in sex discrimination. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 5(3), 665–692. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-8971.5.3.665
    Cited Here
  • Carpinella C. M., Chen J. M., Hamilton D. L., Johnson K. L. (2015). Gendered facial cues influence race categorizations. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 41(3), 405–419. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167214567153
    Cited Here
  • Charlesworth T. E., Yang V., Mann T. C., Kurdi B., Banaji M. R. (2021). Gender stereotypes in natural language: Word embeddings show robust consistency across child and adult language corpora of more than 65 million words. Psychological Science, 32(2), 218–240. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797620963619
    Cited Here
  • Chmielewski M., Kucker S. C. (2020). An MTurk crisis? Shifts in data quality and the impact on study results. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 11(4), 464–473. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550619875149
    Cited Here
  • Clausell E., Fiske S. T. (2005). When do subgroup parts add up to the stereotypic whole? Mixed stereotype content for gay male subgroups explains overall ratings. Social Cognition, 23(2), 161–181. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.23.2.161.65626
    Cited Here
  • Cole E. R. (2009). Intersectionality and research in psychology. The American Psychologist, 64(3), 170–180. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014564
    Cited Here
  • Coles S. M., Pasek J. (2020). Intersectional invisibility revisited: How group prototypes lead to the erasure and exclusion of Black women. Translational Issues in Psychological Science, 6(4), 314–324. https://doi.org/10.1037/tps0000256
    Cited Here
  • Collins P. H. (2015). Intersectionality’s definitional dilemmas. Annual Review of Sociology, 41(1), 1–20. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-soc-073014-112142
    Cited Here
  • Crenshaw K. (1989). Demarginalizing the intersection of race and sex: A black feminist critique of antidiscrimination doctrine, feminist theory and antiracist politics. University of Chicago Legal Forum, 1989(1), 139–167. https://doi.org/https://chicagounbound.uchicago.edu/uclf/vol1989/iss1/8
    Cited Here
  • Das E., Bushman B. J., Bezemer M. D., Kerkhof P., Vermeulen I. E. (2009). How terrorism news reports increase prejudice against outgroups: A terror management account. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(3), 453–459. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2008.12.001
    Cited Here
  • Davis K. (2008). Intersectionality as buzzword: A sociology of science perspective on what makes a feminist theory successful. Feminist Theory, 9(1), 67–85. https://doi.org/10.1177/1464700108086364
    Cited Here
  • Devos T., Banaji M. R. (2005). American = White? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88(3), 447–466. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.88.3.447
    Cited Here
  • Diekman A. B., Goodfriend W. (2006). Rolling with the changes: A role congruity perspective on gender norms. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 30(4), 369–383. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-6402.2006.00312.x
    Cited Here
  • Dupree C. H., Torrez B., Obioha O., Fiske S. T. (2021). Race–status associations: Distinct effects of three novel measures among White and Black perceivers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 120(3), 601–625. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000257
    Cited Here
  • Eagly A. H., Karau S. J. (2002). Role congruity theory of prejudice toward female leaders. Psychological Review, 109(3), 573–598. https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-295X.109.3.573
    Cited Here
  • Eagly A. H., Nater C., Miller D. I., Kaufmann M., Sczesny S. (2020). Gender stereotypes have changed: A cross-temporal meta-analysis of U.S. public opinion polls from 1946 to 2018. American Psychologist, 75(3), 301–315. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000494
    Cited Here
  • Eagly A. H., Steffen V. J. (1984). Gender stereotypes stem from the distribution of women and men into social roles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46(4), 735–754. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.46.4.735
    Cited Here
  • Eagly A. H., Wood W., Diekman A. B. (2000). Social role theory of sex differences and similarities: A current appraisal. In Eckes T., Trautner H. M. (Eds.), The Developmental Social Psychology of Gender ((1st ed, pp. 123–174). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
    Cited Here
  • Else-Quest N. M., Hyde J. S. (2016). Intersectionality in quantitative psychological research: I. theoretical and epistemological issues. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 40(2), 155–170. https://doi.org/10.1177/0361684316629797
    Cited Here
  • Erdfelder E., Faul F., Buchner A. (1996). GPOWER: A general power analysis program. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 28(1), 1–11. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03203630
    Cited Here
  • Fiske S. T., Cuddy A. J. C., Glick P., Xu J. (2002). A model of (often mixed) stereotype content: Competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and competition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(6), 878–902. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.82.6.878
    Cited Here
  • Galinsky A. D., Hall E. V., Cuddy A. J. C. (2013). Gendered races: Implications for interracial marriage, leadership selection, and athletic participation. Psychological Science, 24(4), 498–506. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612457783
    Cited Here
  • Gallagher N. M., Bodenhausen G. V. (2021). Gender essentialism and the mental representation of transgender women and men: A multimethod investigation of stereotype content. Cognition, 217(1), 104887. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104887
    Cited Here
  • Ghavami N., Peplau L. A. (2013). An intersectional analysis of gender and ethnic stereotypes: Testing three hypotheses. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 37(1), 113–127. https://doi.org/10.1177/0361684312464203
    Cited Here
  • Goff P. A., Thomas M. A., Jackson M. C. (2008). Ain’t I a woman?”: Towards an intersectional approach to person perception and group-based harms. Sex Roles, 59(5–6), 392–403. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-008-9505-4
    Cited Here
  • Goh J. X., McCue J. (2021). Perceived prototypicality of Asian subgroups in the United States and the United Kingdom. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 97(1), 104201. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2021.104201
    Cited Here
  • Gündemir S., Homan A. C., De Dreu C. K. W., Van Vugt M. (2014). Think leader, think white? Capturing and weakening an implicit pro-White leadership bias. PLoS ONE, 9(1), e83915. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0083915
    Cited Here
  • Haines E. L., Deaux K., Lofaro N. (2016). The times they are a-changing … or are they not? A comparison of gender stereotypes, 1983–2014. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 40(3), 353–363. https://doi.org/10.1177/0361684316634081
    Cited Here
  • Hall E. V., Galinsky A. D., Phillips K. W. (2015a). Gender profiling: A gendered race perspective on person–position fit. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 41(6), 853–868. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167215580779
    Cited Here
  • Hall E. V., Hall A. V., Galinsky A. D., Phillips K. W. (2019). MOSAIC: A model of stereotyping through associated and intersectional categories. Academy of Management Review, 44(3), 643–672. https://doi.org/10.5465/amr.2017.0109
    Cited Here
  • Hall E. V., Phillips K. W., Glick P., Washington E., Lee S. (2015b). Understanding intersectional challenges: An examination of the status and origins of descriptive and prescriptive gender by race stereotypes.
    Cited Here
  • Hamilton M. C. (1991). Masculine bias in the attribution of personhood: People = male, male = people. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 15(3), 393–402. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-6402.1991.tb00415.x
    Cited Here
  • Harts M. (2020). The memo: What women of color need to know to secure a seat at the table. Seal Press.
    Cited Here
  • Heilman M. E. (2001). Description and prescription: How gender stereotypes prevent women’s ascent up the organizational ladder. Journal of Social Issues, 57(4), 657–674. https://doi.org/10.1111/0022-4537.00234
    Cited Here
  • Hitlin S., Brown J. S., Elder G. H. (2007). Measuring Latinos: Racial vs. ethnic classification and self-understandings. Social Forces, 86(2), 587–611. https://doi.org/10.1093/sf/86.2.587
    Cited Here
  • Johnson K. L., Freeman J. B., Pauker K. (2012). Race is gendered: How covarying phenotypes and stereotypes bias sex categorization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102(1), 116–131. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025335
    Cited Here
  • Johnson K. L., Ghavami N. (2011). At the crossroads of conspicuous and concealable: What race categories communicate about sexual orientation. PLoS ONE, 6(3), e18025. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0018025
    Cited Here
  • Kibria N. (1998). The contested meanings of “Asian American”: Racial dilemmas in the contemporary US. Ethnic and Racial Studies, 21(5), 939–958. https://doi.org/10.1080/014198798329739
    Cited Here
  • Kite M. E., Deaux K. (1987). Gender belief systems: Homosexuality and the implicit inversion theory. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 11(1), 83–96. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-6402.1987.tb00776.x
    Cited Here
  • Koenig A. M., Eagly A. H. (2014). Evidence for the social role theory of stereotype content: Observations of groups’ roles shape stereotypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 107(3), 371–392. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037215
    Cited Here
  • Kuo E. E., Kraus M. W., Richeson J. A. (2020). High-status exemplars and the misperception of the Asian-White wealth gap. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 11(3), 397–405. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550619867940
    Cited Here
  • Lehavot K., Lambert A. J. (2007). Toward a greater understanding of antigay prejudice: On the role of sexual orientation and gender role violation. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 29(3), 279–292. https://doi.org/10.1080/01973530701503390
    Cited Here
  • Lenth R. V. (2022). emmeans: Estimated Marginal Means, aka Least-Squares Means (R package version 1.7.4-1). https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=emmeans
    Cited Here
  • Lick D. J., Johnson K. L. (2016). Straight until proven gay: A systematic bias toward straight categorizations in sexual orientation judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 110(6), 801–817. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000052
    Cited Here
  • Livingston R. W., Pearce N. A. (2009). The teddy-bear effect. Psychological Science, 20(10), 1229–1236. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02431.x
    Cited Here
  • Livingston R. W., Rosette A. S., Washington E. F. (2012). Can an agentic Black woman get ahead? The impact of race and interpersonal dominance on perceptions of female leaders. Psychological Science, 23(4), 354–358. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611428079
    Cited Here
  • Lu A., Wong Y. J. (2013). Stressful Experiences of Masculinity Among U.S.-Born and Immigrant Asian American Men. Gender & Society, 27(3), 345–371. https://doi.org/10.1177/0891243213479446
    Cited Here
  • Neel R., Neufeld S. L., Neuberg S. L. (2013). Would an obese person whistle Vivaldi? Targets of prejudice self-present to minimize appearance of specific threats. Psychological Science, 24(5), 678–687. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612458807
    Cited Here
  • Nicolas G., de la Fuente M., Fiske S. T. (2017). Mind the overlap in multiple categorization: A review of crossed categorization, intersectionality, and multiracial perception. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 20(5), 621–631. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430217708862
    Cited Here
  • Pager D., Western B., Sugie N. (2009). Sequencing disadvantage: Barriers to employment facing young black and white men with criminal records. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 623(1), 195–213. https://doi.org/10.1177/0002716208330793
    Cited Here
  • Pedulla D. S. (2014). The positive consequences of negative stereotypes: Race, sexual orientation, and the job application process. Social Psychology Quarterly, 77(1), 75–94. https://doi.org/10.1177/0190272513506229
    Cited Here
  • Penner A. M., Saperstein A. (2008). How social status shapes race. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105(50), 19628–19630. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0805762105
    Cited Here
  • Penner A. M., Saperstein A. (2013). Engendering racial perceptions: An intersectional analysis of how social status shapes race. Gender & Society, 27(3), 319–344. https://doi.org/10.1177/0891243213480262
    Cited Here
  • Petsko C. D., Bodenhausen G. V. (2019). Racial stereotyping of gay men: Can a minority sexual orientation erase race? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 83(1), 37–54. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2019.03.002
    Cited Here
  • Petsko C. D., Bodenhausen G. V. (2020). Multifarious person perception: How social perceivers manage the complexity of intersectional targets. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 14(2), 1–13. https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12518
    Cited Here
  • Petsko C. D., Rosette A. S., Bodenhausen G. V. (2022). Through the looking glass: A lens-based account of intersectional stereotyping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 123(4), 763–787. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000382
    Cited Here
  • Plant E. A., Devine P. G. (1998). Internal and external motivation to respond without prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(3), 811–832. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.75.3.811
    Cited Here
  • Plant E. A., Goplen J., Kunstman J. W. (2011). Selective responses to threat: The roles of race and gender in decisions to shoot. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37(9), 1274–1281. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167211408617
    Cited Here
  • Preddie J. P., Biernat M. (2021). More than the sum of its parts: Intersections of sexual orientation and race as they influence perceptions of group similarity and stereotype content. Sex Roles, 84(9–10), 554–573. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-020-01185-3
    Cited Here
  • Prentice D. A., Carranza E. (2002). What women and men should be, shouldn’t be, are allowed to be, and don’t have to be: The contents of prescriptive gender stereotypes. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 26(4), 269–281. https://doi.org/10.1111/1471-6402.t01-1-00066
    Cited Here
  • Richardson L. S., Goff P. A. (2014). Interrogating racial violence. Ohio State Journal of Criminal Law, 12(1), 115–152. https://escholarship.org/uc/item/7s73c32x
    Cited Here
  • Ritter B. A., Yoder J. D. (2004). Gender differences in leader emergence persist even for dominant women: An updated confirmation of role congruity theory. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 28(3), 187–193. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-6402.2004.00135.x
    Cited Here
  • Roberts S. O., Weisman K., Lane J. D., Williams A., Camp N. P., Wang M., Robison M., Sanchez K., Griffiths C. (2020). God as a White man: A psychological barrier to conceptualizing Black people and women as leadership worthy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 119(6), 1290–1315. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000233
    Cited Here
  • Rosette A. S., Koval C. Z., Ma A., Livingston R. (2016). Race matters for women leaders: Intersectional effects on agentic deficiencies and penalties. The Leadership Quarterly, 27(3), 1–17. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2016.01.008
    Cited Here
  • Rosette A. S., Leonardelli G. J., Phillips K. W. (2008). The White standard: Racial bias in leader categorization. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93(4), 758–777. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.93.4.758
    Cited Here
  • Sadler M. S., Correll J., Park B., Judd C. M. (2012). The world is not Black and White: Racial bias in the decision to shoot in a multiethnic context. Journal of Social Issues, 68(2), 286–313. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.2012.01749.x
    Cited Here
  • Sandberg S. (2013). Lean in: Women, work, and the will to lead (First edition). Alfred A. Knopf.
    Cited Here
  • Schug J., Alt N. P., Klauer K. C. (2015). Gendered race prototypes: Evidence for the non-prototypicality of Asian men and Black women. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 56(1), 121–125. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2014.09.012
    Cited Here
  • Schug J., Alt N. P., Lu P. S., Gosin M., Fay J. L. (2017). Gendered race in mass media: Invisibility of Asian men and Black women in popular magazines. Psychology of Popular Media Culture, 6(3), 222–236. https://doi.org/10.1037/ppm0000096
    Cited Here
  • Strange A. M., Enos R. D., Hill M., Lakeman A. (2019). Online volunteer laboratories for human subjects research. PLOS ONE, 14(8), e0221676. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0221676
    Cited Here
  • Thomas E. L., Dovidio J. F., West T. V. (2014). Lost in the categorical shuffle: Evidence for the social non-prototypicality of black women. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 20(3), 370–376. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035096
    Cited Here
  • Vial A. C., Brescoll V. L., Dovidio J. F. (2019). Third-party prejudice accommodation increases gender discrimination. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 117(1), 73–98. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000164
    Cited Here
  • Wade R. M., Harper G. W. (2021). Toward a multidimensional construct of racialized sexual discrimination (RSD): Implications for scale development. Psychology of Sexual Orientation and Gender Diversity, 8(4), 401–406. https://doi.org/10.1037/sgd0000443
    Cited Here
  • Warner L. R., Shields S. A. (2013). The intersections of sexuality, gender, and race: Identity research at the crossroads. Sex Roles, 68(11–12), 803–810. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-013-0281-4
    Cited Here
  • Williams J. C. (2014). Double jeopardy? An empirical study with implications for the debates over implicit bias and intersectionality. Harvard Journal of Law and Gender, 37, 185–242. http://repository.uchastings.edu/faculty_scholarship/1278
    Cited Here
  • Williams M. G., Lewis J. A. (2021). Developing a conceptual framework of Black Women’s gendered racial identity development. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 45(2), 212–228. https://doi.org/10.1177/0361684320988602
    Cited Here
Copyright © 2024 Sage Publications
View full text|Download PDF